Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current
    • Ahead of print
    • Past Issues
    • JNM Supplement
    • SNMMI Annual Meeting Abstracts
    • Continuing Education
    • JNM Podcasts
  • Subscriptions
    • Subscribers
    • Institutional and Non-member
    • Rates
    • Journal Claims
    • Corporate & Special Sales
  • Authors
    • Submit to JNM
    • Information for Authors
    • Assignment of Copyright
    • AQARA requirements
  • Info
    • Reviewers
    • Permissions
    • Advertisers
  • About
    • About Us
    • Editorial Board
    • Contact Information
  • More
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Help
    • SNMMI Journals
  • SNMMI
    • JNM
    • JNMT
    • SNMMI Journals
    • SNMMI

User menu

  • Subscribe
  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • My Cart

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of Nuclear Medicine
  • SNMMI
    • JNM
    • JNMT
    • SNMMI Journals
    • SNMMI
  • Subscribe
  • My alerts
  • Log in
  • My Cart
Journal of Nuclear Medicine

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current
    • Ahead of print
    • Past Issues
    • JNM Supplement
    • SNMMI Annual Meeting Abstracts
    • Continuing Education
    • JNM Podcasts
  • Subscriptions
    • Subscribers
    • Institutional and Non-member
    • Rates
    • Journal Claims
    • Corporate & Special Sales
  • Authors
    • Submit to JNM
    • Information for Authors
    • Assignment of Copyright
    • AQARA requirements
  • Info
    • Reviewers
    • Permissions
    • Advertisers
  • About
    • About Us
    • Editorial Board
    • Contact Information
  • More
    • Alerts
    • Feedback
    • Help
    • SNMMI Journals
  • View or Listen to JNM Podcast
  • Visit JNM on Facebook
  • Join JNM on LinkedIn
  • Follow JNM on Twitter
  • Subscribe to our RSS feeds
LetterLetter to the Editor

Reply to “The Value of Functional PET in Quantifying Neurotransmitter Dynamics”

Evan D. Morris, Gaelle M. Emvalomenos, Jocelyn Hoye and Steven R. Meikle
Journal of Nuclear Medicine May 2025, 66 (5) 825-827; DOI: https://doi.org/10.2967/jnumed.125.269473
Evan D. Morris
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Gaelle M. Emvalomenos
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Jocelyn Hoye
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Steven R. Meikle
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

REPLY: We are pleased that Lanzenberger et al. have drawn attention to our recent review, “Modeling PET Data Acquired During Nonsteady Conditions: What If Brain Conditions Change During the Scan?” (1). We are further pleased that our colleagues are advancing methods to push boundaries to find new and more powerful ways to use PET imaging and to explore stimulation-induced changes in the brain. We support it. The letter by Lanzenberger et al. provides us the opportunity to drill down into some important points about transients and the relative strengths of methods.

What is meant by transient? And why is it important? The main focus of our paper was to review and discuss the development of models that include explicit mathematical terms that describe transient phenomena, such as the release of a neurotransmitter in response to a short-lived external stimulus. A transient occurs in the transition from one steady state to another. To describe transients mathematically, we use functions that are time-varying. Thus, the difference between the methods we described in our review and the functional PET (fPET) method the letter-writers refer to is this: The various models detailed (linear simplified reference region model, linear parametric neurotransmitter PET [lp-ntPET], etc.) attempt to describe the dynamics of the neurotransmitter during the transient event and its effect on the PET signal during the transient. fPET, on the other hand, seeks to detect the transition and quantify a difference between 2 states but not to characterize what is happening during the change from one state to another of the PET signal or of the neurotransmitter that caused it.

Does the distinction matter? We believe it does. There are as yet untested hypotheses in the literature that suggest that the transient feeling of “high” is comparable in speed and progression (i.e., curve shape) to the transient dopamine elevation after drug-taking. Thus, it has been speculated that “high” is encoded in the transient dopamine elevation and subsequent return to baseline (2,3). Similarly, there is evidence that reward prediction error during conditioned learning is related to, and thus encoded by, transient dopamine signals (4). And, it is hypothesized that the reward prediction error apparatus is disrupted in schizophrenia (5). In both cases, modeling the transient explicitly (i.e., the time of onset, the peak, and the return to baseline), rather than just detecting it, matters.

What is meant by temporal resolution? In practical terms, it is the ability of a system to resolve distinct phenomena in time. It is not the same as sampling frequency. We have previously established that the practical temporal resolution of lp-ntPET (given a certain realistic sampling frequency, a signal-to-noise ratio, and a particular model) is between 3 and 8 min (6,7). That is, we can reasonably expect to be able to distinguish between one transient dopamine response that peaks 2 min after a stimulus commences (e.g., cigarette smoking) and another one that peaks 7 min after the start of stimulus. Another one of our studies demonstrated that, with additional postprocessing of parametric images, we can reliably differentiate voxel locations tied to an early event from voxel locations tied to a late event (8). In yet an older experimental study, we used another variant of our models (9) (also cited in the review paper) to distinguish between 2 different finger-tapping epochs and correctly classify them as having occurred 15 min apart (10).

Whatever the practical temporal resolution of fPET, it is not seconds, as claimed. Practical temporal resolution can improve with improvements in scanner sensitivity, but as with our work on lp-ntPET and related techniques, the practical temporal resolution can only be established through analysis of simulations and experiments.

What is meant by a ceiling effect? That is, what are the concerns when using a saturable phenomenon as the measurable signal in any assay? The answer must be organized into 2 parts:

  1. Virtually all biologic phenomena are enzyme- or receptor-mediated. All such phenomena are governed by the potential saturation of limited numbers of enzyme- or receptor-binding sites. Biology makes no distinction between a receptor that binds dopamine or an enzyme that synthesizes it. Saturation by itself does not negate the value of a signal, nor does it even undermine its quantifiability. It simply means that the relationship between the input (a drug or a behavioral stimulus) and its measured output (a change in tracer binding to a receptor) is nonlinear.

     What is an investigator to do with a saturable signal? Either configure the experiment to remain squarely in the linear range of the input-output relationship, or apply the appropriate nonlinear model to describe the output as a function of the input. In occupancy studies with PET, the application of Emax models is an example of the latter. In effect, with lp-ntPET and like models, we do both—use nonlinear models and restrict their applicability. Can we use lp-ntPET or other models to differentiate between 2 extreme dopamine levels, both of which cause total blockade of all D2 receptors? No. But in most circumstances, that is not particularly interesting or relevant. All investigators would be well advised that whatever phenomenon they are detecting, it is also likely saturable. Only once one performs a dose–response study can one assess whether saturation has any practical effect on the ability to detect what one cares about.

     Cognitive paradigms are where it’s at. And as stated by the letter-writers, cognitive paradigms yield small signal changes. That is where the field is going—not to detect differences in the effect of 1 truckload of amphetamine versus that of 2 truckloads, but rather to detect and characterize the responses to subtle behavioral and cognitive challenges.

  2. From where does the claim of a ceiling effect of 40% signal change come, and what does it mean for the utility of binding potential? This is worth addressing—and laying to rest. The observations, originally published as early as 1997 (11,12), report that the change in binding potential (ΔBP) of raclopride in response to large doses of amphetamine tops out at 40%. This has somehow been enshrined in the PET literature as a demonstration of a fatal weakness of ΔBP as a measure of neurotransmitter response, broadly.

     First, consider the above arguments about the use of linear versus nonlinear signals. Second, we must always be mindful of what binding potential is. It is a constant and not a function of time. Thus, it only truly applies to the description of a system at steady state. In turn, the assumption, when using ΔBP to assay for the effect of a dopaminergic drug, is that synaptic dopamine level transitions abruptly from one steady state (baseline) to another state (drug-induced) where it remains for the duration of the scan or extended measurement period. This is a very restrictive idealization. Transitions happen over finite amounts of time and have varied arcs. The ramifications of applying ΔBP when the system does not jump instantaneously or consistently from one steady state to another, or worse, when a second steady state is never reached, have been well studied and explained by Yoder et al. (13) and Sullivan et al. (14) and reprised in our review. Any PET user who persists in defying prevailing assumptions of an analysis method must proceed at his or her own risk.

In summary, models such as lp-ntPET using an explicit transient term do not labor under the assumption of instantaneous transition between steady states. Rather, they embrace the transition and model it. Used appropriately, they can uncover important patterns and phenomena that have direct relevance to the neurobiology of reinforcement learning and drug addiction and that have, heretofore, been largely ignored by the PET research community. Naturally, we welcome all new techniques that have the potential to extract more information from dynamic PET data. To characterize transient neurotransmitter responses to drugs and behavior, however, the class of models and techniques catalogued in our recent review paper are the current best efforts to do so.

DISCLOSURE

No potential conflict of interest relevant to this article was reported.

Evan D. Morris*, Gaelle M. Emvalomenos, Jocelyn Hoye, Steven R. Meikle

*Yale University New Haven, Connecticut

E-mail: evan.morris{at}yale.edu

Footnotes

  • Published online Mar. 13, 2025.

  • © 2025 by the Society of Nuclear Medicine and Molecular Imaging.

REFERENCES

  1. 1.↵
    1. Morris ED,
    2. Emvalomenos GM,
    3. Hoye J,
    4. Meikle SR
    . Modeling PET data acquired during nonsteady conditions: what if brain conditions change during the scan? J Nucl Med. 2024;65:1824–1837.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. 2.↵
    1. Volkow ND,
    2. Ding Y,
    3. Fowler JS,
    4. et al
    . Is methylphenidate like cocaine? studies on their pharmacokinetics and distribution in human brain. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52:456–463.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. 3.↵
    1. Volkow ND,
    2. Swanson JM
    . Variables that affect the clinical use and abuse of methylphenidate in the treatment of ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:1909–1918.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. 4.↵
    1. Saunders BT,
    2. Richard JM,
    3. Margolis EB,
    4. et al
    . Dopamine neurons create Pavlovian conditioned stimuli with circuit-defined motivational properties. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:1072–1083.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. 5.↵
    1. Millard SJ,
    2. Bearden CE,
    3. Karlsgodt KH,
    4. et al
    . The prediction-error hypothesis of schizophrenia: new data point to circuit-specific changes in dopamine activity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:628–640.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. 6.↵
    1. Normandin MD,
    2. Morris ED
    . Temporal resolution of ntPET using either arterial or reference region-derived plasma input functions. 2006 International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society; New York. 2006:2005–2008.
  7. 7.↵
    1. Normandin MD,
    2. Schiffer WK,
    3. Morris ED
    . A linear model for estimation of neurotransmitter response profiles from dynamic PET data. Neuroimage. 2012;59:2689–2699.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Liu H,
    2. Morris ED
    . Detecting and classifying neurotransmitter signals from ultra-high sensitivity PET data: the future of molecular brain imaging. Phys Med Biol. 2021;66:175007.
    OpenUrl
  9. 9.↵
    1. Constantinescu CC,
    2. Bouman C,
    3. Morris ED
    . Nonparametric extraction of transient changes in neurotransmitter concentration from dynamic PET data. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2007;26:359–373.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  10. 10.↵
    1. Morris ED,
    2. Constantinescu CC,
    3. Sullivan JM,
    4. Normandin MD,
    5. Christopher LA
    . Noninvasive visualization of human dopamine dynamics from PET images. Neuroimage. 2010;51:135–144.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  11. 11.↵
    1. Drevets W,
    2. Price J,
    3. Kupfer D,
    4. et al
    . PET Measures of amphetamine-induced dopamine release in ventral versus dorsal striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1999;21:694–709.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. 12.↵
    1. Hartvig P,
    2. Torstenson R,
    3. Tedroff J,
    4. et al
    . Amphetamine effects on dopamine release and synthesis rate studied in the Rhesus monkey brain by positron emission tomography. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 1997;104:329–339.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  13. 13.↵
    1. Yoder KK,
    2. Wang C,
    3. Morris ED
    . Change in binding potential as a quantitative index of neurotransmitter release is highly sensitive to relative timing and kinetics of the tracer and the endogenous ligand. J Nucl Med. 2004;45:903–911.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  14. 14.↵
    1. Sullivan JM,
    2. Kim SJ,
    3. Cosgrove KP,
    4. Morris ED
    . Limitations of SRTM, Logan graphical method, and equilibrium analysis for measuring transient dopamine release with [11C]raclopride PET. Am J Nucl Med Mol Imaging. 2013;3:247–260.
    OpenUrlPubMed
  • Received for publication February 20, 2025.
  • Accepted for publication February 20, 2025.
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Journal of Nuclear Medicine: 66 (5)
Journal of Nuclear Medicine
Vol. 66, Issue 5
May 1, 2025
  • Table of Contents
  • Table of Contents (PDF)
  • About the Cover
  • Index by author
  • Complete Issue (PDF)
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on Journal of Nuclear Medicine.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Reply to “The Value of Functional PET in Quantifying Neurotransmitter Dynamics”
(Your Name) has sent you a message from Journal of Nuclear Medicine
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the Journal of Nuclear Medicine web site.
Citation Tools
Reply to “The Value of Functional PET in Quantifying Neurotransmitter Dynamics”
Evan D. Morris, Gaelle M. Emvalomenos, Jocelyn Hoye, Steven R. Meikle
Journal of Nuclear Medicine May 2025, 66 (5) 825-827; DOI: 10.2967/jnumed.125.269473

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Reply to “The Value of Functional PET in Quantifying Neurotransmitter Dynamics”
Evan D. Morris, Gaelle M. Emvalomenos, Jocelyn Hoye, Steven R. Meikle
Journal of Nuclear Medicine May 2025, 66 (5) 825-827; DOI: 10.2967/jnumed.125.269473
Twitter logo Facebook logo LinkedIn logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One
Bookmark this article

Jump to section

  • Article
    • DISCLOSURE
    • Footnotes
    • REFERENCES
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • No citing articles found.
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Maintaining the Evidence for In Vivo Brain Estrogen Receptor Density by Neuroendocrine Aging and Relationships with Cognition and Symptomatology
  • Limitations of Retrospective Study Design and Potential Bias in the PRECISE-MDT Study
Show more Letter to the Editor

Similar Articles

SNMMI

© 2025 SNMMI

Powered by HighWire